新加坡六合彩开奖直播

 

Cracking the "coda": What Pacific sperm whales tell us about culture in the animal kingdom

- September 8, 2015

A sperm whale dives under the waves. (Provided photos)
A sperm whale dives under the waves. (Provided photos)

Observing cultural evolution: it may not be something you鈥檇 expect from a marine biologist but 新加坡六合彩开奖直播鈥檚 Mauricio Cantor, a PhD candidate in biology, is zeroing in on the cultures of the deep.

Since 2013, Cantor, under the supervision of studied sperm whales off the Gal谩pagos Islands. His paper, published this week in Nature Communications, details findings that suggest culture鈥攇enerally viewed as distinctly human鈥攊s also at play among these marine giants.

鈥淐ulture in animal societies is a highly debated topic,鈥 says Cantor, originally from Curitiba, Brazil. 鈥淪ome experts think it鈥檚 clear enough, while others don鈥檛 think the word 鈥榗ulture鈥 should be used describing anything but humans.鈥

Cantor鈥檚 study examined two clans of sperm whales, observed in the Pacific since the 鈥80s, that share the same geographic area. Despite living in the same neighbourhood, each clan of whales developed their own 鈥渄ialects鈥 composed of聽 patterns of clicking sounds called codas.

聽鈥淚t鈥檚 quite rare,鈥 says Cantor, explaining that two clans of the same species sharing an area don鈥檛 typically develop unique communication signals. But this kind of segregation is evident in human populations and, broadly speaking, comes as a result of cultural evolution. With that in mind, Cantor set out to find evidence that culture isn鈥檛 just a feature of human life.

Conversations of the deep


Funded by The National Geographic Society, Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC), the Killam Trusts and The Brazilian National Council for Scientific and Technological Development (CNPq), Cantor and Whitehead teamed up with the Gal谩pagos National Park and began exploring the deep waters.

The team listened for the whales鈥 clicking sounds with an underwater microphone to nail down their locations and study their coda repertoires. Once the whales came to the surface to breathe, about every 40 minutes, the team photographed their tails. The ridge of the tail is a sperm whale鈥檚 equivalent to a fingerprint. The team matched codas and behaviours to individual sperm whales.

After the fieldwork was complete, Cantor and his colleagues combined that data with previous Whitehead Lab data dating as far back as the 鈥80s to create virtual populations of whales using computer simulations.

鈥淲e try to backtrack the patterns we observe in the wild to infer how the clan segregation could have evolved,鈥 says Cantor. 鈥淭he computer will simulate the life of several sperm whale populations that acquire codas in different ways over thousands of years. At the end, we see which case could produce clans with different dialects.鈥

Determining dialects


Looking to investigate how the whales created two separate dialects, Cantor simulated several scenarios with these virtual populations. One examined individual learning: how the dialects would evolve if the whales created codas by themselves. Another examined genetic transmission: how the dialects would evolve if they innately knew which codas to produce. A third examined social learning: when whales could listen and copy the codas they hear from other whales. Social learning could be biased; for instance, whales could conform to learn the most common codas, or learn from the individuals who produce similar codas, or use specific codas to mark the identity of the clans they belong to.

鈥淥ur findings show that biased social learning is a required ingredient for the segregation of clans of sperm whales with different 鈥榙ialects鈥,鈥 says Cantor. Through the simulations, he explains, it becomes evident that whales are learning how to communicate from each other 鈥 similar to how humans work. The whales in each clan are conforming to those most similar around them.

鈥淭his gives us evidence that key features of human culture 鈥 which we think makes us so different from everything else in nature 鈥 might be at play in populations of other animals,鈥 says Cantor. 鈥淢aybe we鈥檙e not as different as we thought.鈥

Culture connections


Showing that culture can be a mechanism structuring animal societies, Cantor鈥檚 work will fuel the ongoing debate among experts all over the world. Now tourists in the world-famous islands will have more insight into this debate too. Through the partnership with the Gal谩pagos National Park, Cantor provided material that helps guides explain sperm whale behaviour and culture to visitors.

"I鈥檝e been trying to understand the culture of sperm whales for quite a few years, and for even more I鈥檝e studied their societies,鈥 says Dr. Whitehead, biology professor at 新加坡六合彩开奖直播 and Cantor鈥檚 supervisor. 鈥淲hat Cantor has done is show how interrelated sperm whale society and sperm whale culture are, and how the feedbacks between culture and society may have worked in the evolution of this extraordinary animal."

The Whitehead Lab is known for its work examining culture in marine life. Late last year, Dr. Whitehead and Dr. Luke Rendell of the University of St. Andrews, a former PhD student in the Whitehead Lab, co-authored a book titled The Cultural Lives of Dolphins and Whales. .